Research output per year
Research output per year
The efforts of our ongoing studies are to describe observed early atypical behaviours
during infancy, especially those that may be associated with autism spectrum conditions
(ASCs) in order to make it possible to start an early support intervention.
During the last decade a number of studies about early screening of ASCs have been
published. Mainly they have used two types of sampling, either a general screening
tool followed by ASC-specific instruments/observations/interviews for children
scoring positive at the first line, or alternatively, an ASC-specific tool for children in
the risk zone (sibling or twin studies) (Pinto-Martin et al., 2008). The number of
studies is quantifiable and also includes studies using a randomised controlled experimental
design (Dawson et al., 2010). However, screening studies on the basis of a
given population (Nygren et al., 2012) in primary health care or at paediatric clinics/
university institutions are sparsely reported in scientific publications leaving us
without reliable knowledge of ASC prevalence in very young authentic populations.
Recently Bolton, Golding, Emond, and Steer (2012) published a longitudinal and
cross-sectional study of early predictors of later ASCs and autistic traits. Results
indicated that in the first year differences in attention, inhibition of control and vocalisations
were striking in comparison with typically developed infants (Landa, Gross,
Stuart, & Faherty, 2013).
The American Academy of Pediatrics, in its Embargo of 2006, claims that early
identification of children at risk of ASCs is urgently needed for early intervention.
The reasoning behind that statement is the relative plasticity of the developing brain
during a child’s first years and also the increasing amount of knowledge of early
signs and markers of ASC-specific behaviours (American Psychiatric Association,
2013; Barbaro & Dissanayake, 2013; Barbaro, Ridgway, & Dissanayake, 2011;
Crane & Winsler, 2008; Daniels, Halladay, Shih, Elder, & Dawson, 2014; Dereu
et al., 2010; Gura, Champagne, & Blood-Siegfried, 2011), atypical child development
(Ozonoff et al., 2008) and medical status associated with ASCs (Eriksson et al., 2013;
Karmel et al., 2010). The problem of regression (Ozonoff et al., 2010; Toth, Munson,
Meltzoff, & Dawson, 2006) during the second year of life for about 20% of children
later diagnosed with an ASC has been highlighted as a contra argument (Webb,
Dawson, Bernier, & Panagiotides, 2006) towards a consequence of too-early screening.
This argument is relevant for national child health-care programmes that follow infants
only few times during childhood, but if one has a continuous programme up until
school age, these children would show up in an ASC programme later anyway.
Although the risk of false-positive findings exists, most parents prefer the stress and
worry of an early positive identification in contrast to a missed opportunity of an
early intervention (Barton, Dumont-Mathieu, & Fein, 2012). However, it is a
problem to distinguish children with other developmental concerns from children
with ASCs at around 12 months of age (Manning-Courtney et al., 2013).
Inglese (2009) emphasised the need for nurses in the community and primary care
settings to improve their knowledge about ASCs and to learn to screen for behaviours
associated with ASCs in children due to the fact that they will encounter an increasing
number of children with these conditions. Cangialose and Allen (2014) emphasise the
support from recent research that ASC-specific behaviours and developmental delays
emerge early in infancy and that an early screening can contribute the identification
of infants in the risk zone of an ASC. Eriksson et al. (2013) recently reported from a
Swedish intervention programme where toddlers (20–54 months) received intervention
care based on principles of non-intensive or intensive applied behaviour analysis. The
result indicated that toddlers without epilepsy and intellectual disorder (ID) gained
more positive effects of the intervention in comparison with children with epilepsy
and ID. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition
(DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) is the most widely accepted nomenclature
used by clinicians and researchers for the classification of mental disorders
manual focuses on two diagnostic domains: social communication deficits and
restricted and stereotyped behaviours. Deficits in social skills and delays in language
development are probably the two most reliable aspects of very early identification
of ASCs, but a social communication delay must not obligatorily refer to an ASC diagnosis
and is not sufficient for ASC identification. Moreover, there is also a need for
identification of restricted and/or stereotyped behaviours. It may be difficult to find
reliable behaviours of these ranges during infancy.
An enlightening perspective among both clinicians and researchers is that ASCs
often are associated with other medical problems (Carbone, 2013; Konopka, 2013).
Teitelbaum, Teitelbaum, Nye, Fryman, and Maurer (1998) used a movement analysis
system to compare movements (lying, righting oneself, sitting, crawling and standing)
of infants with an ASC with typically developed infants at the ages of four and six
months. They concluded that movement disturbances are clearly detectable at this
early age. Ozonoff et al. (2010) found indications of an overall slowed rate of motor
development for infants with an ASC compared with typically developed infants. Seizures
have also been observed during the first few days of life and indicate a more
serious development of an ASC (Eriksson et al., 2013). At one month of age a
higher percentage of infants later diagnosed with an ASC displayed asymmetric
visual tracking and abnormal upper extremity tone, and at four months they showed
a greater preference for higher frequency stimulation when less aroused as well as performing
more like newborns than four-month-old infants (Karmel et al., 2010). At six
months of age, Chawarska, Macari, and Shic (2013) found that infants later diagnosed
with an ASC paid less attention to the social scene and were more diminished in their
ability to react spontaneously to people than typically developed infants. Atypical patterns
of electroencephalography activity have been demonstrated by Dawson et al.
(2002) in young children with an ASC. They have shown that early intervention
with the Early-Start Denver Model in a randomised, controlled trial design study
resulted in increased cortical activation in the ASC group of children that viewed
faces in comparison with children with an ASC group treated as usual. Research has
also been directed at infants’ attention capacity in order to study both audio-visual
and visual stimulation and its effects on memory (Flom & Bahrick, 2010) as this is
essential for typical psychological development of infants.
Screening for ASCs in primary health-care settings was performed by Robins as
early as 2008 by using the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-Chat) and
following the M-Chat with an interview (Robins, 2008). The M-Chat instrument is a
further development of the original British instrument the CHAT (Baron-Cohen,
Allen, & Gillberg, 1992). In recent publications, Robins et al. (2014) and Chlebowski,
Robins, Barton, and Fein (2013) have advanced the M-Chat R into R/F for toddlers
between the ages of 16 and 30 months in a large sample (n = 16,071; 18,989, respectively)
of parents/toddlers in the American city of Atlanta and state Connecticut. Results
indicated that 47.5% of the children having 3 or higher CHAT points were in the risk
zone of being diagnosed with an ASC. A recent Swedish study (Nygren et al., 2012)
completed with the M-Chat and the joint attention observation Five-Item Observational
Scale with an additional brief speech/language assessment scale have been performed
in primary health-care settings with children of the ages 30–34 months. Nurses
observed children’s competencies of joint attention at 2.5 years of age. Sixty-four children
(of 3999) were purportedly autistic and 48 were confirmed with an ASC diagnosis.
Other studies have been performed with infants and toddlers in primary health-care settings
or community-based settings. Barbaro et al. (2011) conducted a screening study in
Australia using paediatric nurses in the first level of implementation of the developmental
surveillance programme with the purpose of lowering the age at which ASCs are
diagnosed in order to start an intervention an earlier age. The conclusion of the
study was that routine monitoring done by nurses for ASCs should be standardised
in relation to what it is now. Barbaro and Dissanayake (2013) reported a new community-
based study from Australia with the focus of determining the most discriminating
and predictive markers of ASC. The Social Attention and Communication Scale was
used at 12, 18 and 24 months of age for three groups: children with autistic disorder,
ASC and developmental and/or language delay. Deficits in eye contact with finger
pointing were consistent across the ages for the groups of autistic disorder and
ASCs, however at the age of 24 months pretend play was the best marker.
In a study from the Netherlands (Oosterling et al., 2010) using the Early Screening
of Autistic Traits Questionnaire the aims were to develop and evaluate a two-step
screening approach for early detection of ASCs. Results showed that in the experimental
study group ASCs were diagnosed 21 months earlier than in the control group and
with the mean age at ASC diagnosis decreasing by 19.5 months. Children from the
experimental group were 9.4 times more likely to be diagnosed before 36 months
than the controls. Pinto-Martin et al. (2008) conducted a study comparing the
number of children (18–30-month-olds) identified at risk for ASCs visiting primary
paediatric care using a general screening tool (Parents’ Evaluation of Developmental
Status) and an autism-specific tool (M-Chat). Findings indicated that a parent-based
tool was not sufficient in comparison with an autism-specific tool. However, the
sample was too small for any far-reaching conclusions to be made.
In 2015, UN member states agreed to 17 global Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs) to end poverty, protect the planet and ensure prosperity for all. This person’s work contributes towards the following SDG(s):
Research output: Contribution to journal › Article › peer-review
Research output: Contribution to journal › Article › peer-review
Research output: Contribution to journal › Article › peer-review
Research output: Contribution to journal › Article › peer-review
Research output: Contribution to journal › Article › peer-review
Jakobsson, U. (Supervisor), Carlsson Höglund, L. (Research student), Lundqvist, P. (Assistant supervisor) & Sivberg, B. (Assistant supervisor)
2022/01/01 → …
Project: Research
Kristensson Hallström, I. (Researcher), Kvist, L. (Researcher), Dykes, A.-K. (Researcher), Erlandsson, L.-K. (Researcher), Bergström, S. (Researcher), Edwinson Månsson, M. (Researcher), Berggren, V. (Researcher), Pålsson, P. (Researcher), Ekelin, M. (Researcher), Persson, E.-K. (Researcher), Landgren, K. (Researcher), Hegaard, H. K. (Researcher), Skoog, M. (Researcher), Derwig, M. (Researcher), Björk, J. (Researcher), Lefevre, Å. (Researcher), Sivberg, B. (Researcher) & Quaye, A. A. (Research student)
Public Health Agency of Sweden
2013/01/01 → 2021/12/31
Project: Research
Kristensson Hallström, I. (PI), Magnusson, B. (Researcher), Lindgren, B. (Researcher), Hansson, K. (Researcher), Hegaard, H. K. (Researcher), Tiberg, I. (Researcher), Landgren, K. (Researcher), Shargie, M. B. (Researcher), Carlsson, A. (Researcher), Kvist, L. (Researcher), Ekelin, M. (Researcher), Derwig, M. (Researcher), Björk, J. (Researcher), Steen Carlsson, K. (Researcher), Skoog, M. (Researcher), Lefevre, Å. (Researcher), Hansson, H. (Researcher), Persson, E.-K. (Researcher), Pålsson, P. (Researcher), Jerene, D. (Researcher), Idvall, M. (Researcher), Bergström, S. (Researcher), Johnsson, B. A. (Researcher), Dykes, A.-K. (Researcher), Erlandsson, L.-K. (Researcher), Edwinson Månsson, M. (Researcher), Berggren, V. (Researcher), Schmiegelow, K. (Researcher), Selander, B. (Researcher), Castor, A. (Researcher), Nilsson, G. (Researcher), Nordmark, E. (Researcher), Sjöström-Strand, A. (Researcher), Gårdling, J. (Researcher), Måstrup, R. (Researcher), Lundqvist, P. (Researcher), Castor, C. (Researcher), Sivberg, B. (Researcher), Quaye, A. A. (Research student) & Persson, M. C. (Administrator)
The Swedish Childhood Cancer Fund, Diabetesfonden - Stiftelsen Svenska Diabetesförbundets Forskningsfond, Ekhagastiftelsen, County Administrative Board, Östergötlands län, Swedish Research Council, Region Skåne, Crafoord Foundation, Fanny Ekdahls Stiftelse, Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (Forte)
2013/01/01 → 2021/12/31
Project: Research